January 4, 2004
Dear Friend of the Cognitive Science Journal,
In keeping with the last three years’ tradition, I am sending this annual
editor’s report to the extended Cognitive Science family which includes
reviewers, authors, editorial board members, and some selected other members
of the cognitive science community. This report describes some recent changes
to the journal and presents relevant statistics for 2004.
1) Changes and Updates
1A) Board changes
2005 will be the last year for the current editorial board. Our stint will be
over on December 31, although some of the editorial board will most likely be
asked to continue with the next editor. For authors this means that the next
couple of months would be an excellent time to submit your manuscripts to us
if you want to avoid getting caught between editorial boards. For potential
editors, it is not too late to submit a nomination for the new editor of Cognitive
Science. Here is the call for nominations:
The governing board of the Cognitive Science Society has opened nominations, including self-nominations, for the editorship of Cognitive Science. Candidates should have broad knowledge of cognitive science, and uphold the Cognitive Science Society’s goals of promoting an interdisciplinary science of minds and other intelligent systems. Desirable attributes for an editor include fairness, demonstrated editorial acumen, a strong research record, timeliness, diligence, dedication, and good judgment. The candidate should be available to receive manuscripts January 1, 2006. The initial term for the editor is three years, with an option to extend the editorship by an additional term. The governing board encourages participation by members of underrepresented groups in the publication process and particularly welcomes such nominations.
To nominate a candidate, prepare a support statement in one page or less and address it to the chair of the search committee: Douglas Medin. Informal inquiries can be emailed to medin@northwestern.edu. Nominations should be sent to the executive officer of the society, Thomas Ward, at editor@cognitivesciencesociety.org. The first review of nominations will begin February 1, 2005. Please submit nominations before then to assure full consideration.
In the meantime, the 2005 board will closely resemble the 2004 board, with only a few changes. The 2005 editorial board will be:
Associate Editors
John R. Anderson, Carnegie Mellon University
Nick Chater, University of Warwick
Marvin M. Chun, Yale University
Andy Clark, University of Edinburgh
Eve V. Clark, Stanford University
Gary S. Dell, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign
Shimon Edelman, Cornell University
Kenneth Forbus, Northwestern University
Dedre Gentner, Northwestern University
James Greeno, University of Pittsburgh
Dan Sperber, Institut Jean Nicod, CNRS, EHESS, and ENS, Paris
Joshua B. Tenenbaum, Massachusetts Institute of Technology
Board of Reviewers
Woo-kyoung Ahn, Yale University
Peter C-H. Cheng, University of Sussex
Susan Epstein, Hunter College and The Graduate School of the City University of New York
Cynthia L. Fisher, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign
Robert M. French, Université de Bourgogne
Adele E. Goldberg, Princeton University
Susan Goldin-Meadow, University of Chicago
Charles Goodwin, University of California, Los Angeles
Arthur Graesser, University of Memphis
Wayne D. Gray, Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute
Ray Jackendoff, Brandeis University
Frank C. Keil, Yale University
Stephen M. Kosslyn, Harvard University
Barbara Landau, Johns Hopkins University
Pat Langley, ISLE and Stanford University
Michael D. Lee, University of Adelaide
Richard L. Lewis, University of Michigan
Arthur B. Markman, University of Texas, Austin
Raymond J. Mooney, University of Texas, Austin
Terry Regier, University of Chicago
Steven Sloman, Brown University
Keith Stenning, The University of Edinburgh
James Tanaka, University of Victoria, British Columbia
Paul Thagard, University of Waterloo
Michael Tomasello, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology
Janet Wiles, The University of Queensland
1B) Special issues
Since 2001, the David E. Rumelhart Prize has been awarded annually to an individual
or collaborative team making a significant contemporary contribution to the
formal analysis of human cognition. The journal has instituted a policy of honoring
Rumelhart prize winners by developing special issues of the journal around the
prize winner’s research area and/or colleagues. In 2003 and 2004 we had
special issues honoring the second and third Rumelhart Prize winners, Richard
Shiffrin and Aravind Joshi. In 2005, Issue #3 will honor the fourth Rumelhart
prize recipient, John Anderson. This issue will focus on recent advances in
computational models using unified cognitive architectures such as ACT-R. The
special issue authors include Rick Lewis, Marsha Lovett, Peter Pirolli, Dario
Salvucci, Niels Taatgen, and of course Dr. Anderson himself. The fifth Rumelhart
prize recipient is Paul Smolensky, and plans are afoot to have a special issue
honoring him in 2006.
1C) Change of publisher
The Cognitive Science Society has signed a multi-year contract with Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates to publish Cognitive Science starting with the first issue
of 2005. The journal has never been published by LEA before, but in many ways,
our relocation feels like a home coming. In 1976, Roger Schank approached Larry
Erlbaum with the idea of forming a cognitive science journal. Mr. Erlbaum was
highly receptive, and together with his partner Walter Johnson began publishing
Cognitive Science in 1977 at the newly established Ablex Publishing Company.
In the intervening years, LEA has published the Annual Proceedings of the Cognitive
Science Conference. Beyond this, LEA has been a major force for the advancement
of cognitive science through the impressive collection of books and journals
they have published in the field. We are honored and delighted to be working
with LEA. Lawrence Erlbaum, both the man and the company, has been exceptionally
generous with his time, expertise, and support. Indeed, his contributions to
cognitive science have been recognized twice by the Cognitive Science Society,
as two independent governing boards have made him an honorary member of the
society. As of August 2004, Lawrence Erlbaum has an official position on the
Cognitive Science Society’s finances committee, but this position only
formally acknowledges his long-time contributions to the society’s financial
and intellectual well-being. We have every expectation that the journal will
flourish with LEA. Through resources provided by LEA, the Cognitive Science
Society will be able to significantly grow, offering its members new services
for reasonable dues. LEA is also committed to the timely production of issues,
Cognitive Science Society’s continued retention of copyrights, authors’
right to disseminate their articles on their own web sites, and continued growth
of the journal.
2) Statistics (thanks to Caroline Verdier for assembling the critical data)
2A) Submissions
Submission rates to the journal continue to rise, reaching an all-time high
level. In 2000, 2001, 2002, 2003, and 2004 we had 63, 126, 174, 188, and 210
submissions respectively. This increase will not come as a surprise to many
of you on the editorial board. This increase, as welcome as it is as far as
indicating the growing popularity of the journal, has certainly taxed our editorial
and reviewer resources. Out of the 210 submissions in 2004, 136 were new manuscripts,
and the remaining were resubmissions of previously submitted manuscripts. Of
the 74 resubmissions, 40 were resubmissions of a manuscript previously given
a “revise and resubmit” designation, and 30 were revisions from
“accept with revision” designations, and 4 were prior “rejects.”
Our number of new manuscripts only increased by 3 from 133 in 2003 to 136 in
2004. However, our resubmissions increased from 55 to 74. This is a natural
demographic shift given that the current editorial board has been on duty for
several years now, and several manuscripts have been wending their way toward
the light of publication.
There are four major kinds of submissions: letters to the editor with about
1,000 words, brief reports with about 4,000 words, regular articles with about
12,000 words, and extended articles with about 18,000 words. Out of the 210
submissions in 2004, 2 were letters, 59 were brief reports, 115 were regular
articles, and 34 were extended articles. The proportions of brief report in
2001, 2002, 2003, and 2004 were 18%, 26%, 29%, and 28% respectively. These numbers
suggest that the word is finally out, and that people think of us when preparing
brief, as well as regular-length, manuscripts.
The 2004 editorial board consisted of 12 associate editors and 25 review board
members. I personally handled 54 manuscripts in 2004, and the associate editors
handled approximately 13 manuscripts each. Each review board member reviewed
an average of 8 manuscripts, and we had 214 ad hoc reviewers, many of whom reviewed
multiple manuscripts. The increased submission rate has put additional strains
on our editorial team, but I have been grateful and proud of their professionalism,
thoughtfulness, and generosity in providing intelligent judgments and comments
on manuscripts.
2B) Acceptance Rates
For accounting purposes, we classify every manuscript into one of four categories
after an editor has acted on it: accept, accept with revision, revise and resubmit,
and reject. Grouping the first two categories together, and counting each submission
separately (i.e., counting a resubmission of a manuscript as a separate submission),
the journal had an acceptance rate of 21% for manuscripts submitted in 2001,
28% in 2002, 29% in 2003, and 38% for the 158 manuscripts submitted in 2004
for which decisions have already been made. These acceptance rates include submissions
for special issues, and so are somewhat artificially elevated. Although it seems
as though our acceptance rate has increased dramatically from last year, this
is because in 2004 we received a much larger proportion of revised manuscripts,
and revisions have a substantially greater probability of being accepted. Also,
all of these probabilities are elevated because many manuscripts are first conditionally
accepted and then finally accepted. For this reason, a more appropriate way
of calculating the probability of acceptance is to track each manuscript longitudinally.
Using this method, the probability of a manuscript eventually being published
is approximately 30% including invited special issue submissions, or approximately
24% if special issue submissions are ignored.
In 2002, there was an apparent trend for brief articles to have a lower acceptance
rate (13%) than regular articles (35%) or extended articles (35%). This trend
was not at all apparent in 2003, with brief reports, regular articles, and extended
articles having acceptance rates of 29%, 24%, and 26% respectively. In 2004,
the trend has reversed, with acceptance rates for brief, regular, and extended
submissions at 43%, 34%, and 36% respectively. This may be another indication
that brief reports have finally been accepted as part of the norm for the journal,
by both authors and editors alike.
2C) Lag Times
The editorial lag time is defined as the number of days between when the on-line
submission software registers a submission as complete (when the authors have
supplied the manuscript, keywords, and manuscript designation) and when the
software sends the author the completed editorial decision, action letter, and
reviews. The table below shows the average lags in days:
Article Type | 2001 | 2002 | 2003 | 2004 |
Brief Reports | 75 | 49 | 75 | 46 |
Regular Articles | 111 | 79 | 76 | 62 |
Extended Articles | 142 | 107 | 106 | 71 |
Letters | n.a. | 38 | 47 | n.a. |
The average editorial lag was 59.4 days. For the first year since I have been
editor, in 2004 our average lag times for all submission categories were within
our target ranges. For this accomplishment, I can only thank the associate editors
and reviewers for their diligence and timeliness.
Another kind of lag that is relevant is the lag to print. We calculate print
lag time as the number of days intervening between when we receive the final
version of the manuscript to two weeks after the scheduled dispatch date (a
date by which most people have received the journal). The overall average is
156 days, broken down by article category as follows:
Brief reports (9 manuscripts): 156 days
Regular articles (22 manuscripts): 162 days
Extended articles (3 manuscripts): 139 days
Although these lags may seem long, they are quite favorable compared to other
journals, and are largely attributable to copy editing, assembling supplemental
annex materials, final author changes, and author proofing. These lags are only
minimally due to the backlog of articles waiting to be printed. Our average
backlog for 2004 was only about 9 articles, up only slightly from 2003’s
backlog of 6 articles. Another factor to consider is that the articles from
2004 were available on-line at Elsevier’s Science Direct service well
before they appeared in hard-copy form. We will have a similar arrangement in
2005 with LEA using their on-line service. After an author has proofread the
PDF file created by the copy editor, it appears on-line to all members of the
Cognitive Science Society, or any member of an institution with an on-line subscription
to the journal. If you have a personal subscription to the journal, then you
can access articles in press at http://www.leaonline.com/.
This site will also direct you to PDF versions of all published articles from
1995-present. LEA will soon be sending members of the Cognitive Science Society
instructions on how to access Cognitive Science articles via LEA-Online. In
addition, you can access articles from 1995-2004 via Elsevier’s site at
http://www.elsevier.com/gej-ng/10/15/15/show/.
Another indication of our journal’s efficiency is provided by author comments.
Andreas Ortmann and Peter Todd have put together an independent web site they
call “Academic Journals Feedback Project” that allows authors to
post their own comments about journals specializing in economics and psychology:
http://www.cerge-ei.cz/multiversity/JFeedBack/index.asp.
On this main page, if you select “psychology” as the field and press
“submit” you will be able to look up authors’ feedback for
Cognitive Science and many of its peer journals. By my admittedly biased eye,
authors are fairly positive about their dealings with Cognitive Science.
2D) Topic Areas
We ask all of our associate editors and members of the board of reviewers to
supply keywords for their domains of expertise and we also ask authors to supply
keywords that fit their manuscripts. Rather than try to define whether cognitive
science is characterized by its contributing set of fields, content topics,
or methods, we include all three classification schemes in our list of keywords.
The table below represents counts of individual members of the 2005 editorial
board (listed as “Board”) and submitted manuscripts from 2001-2004
in the right 4 columns. Thus, for example, 14 members of the editorial board
(out of a total of 39 board members) responded that they would be comfortable
reviewing manuscripts in the field of artificial intelligence, and in 2003 and
2004 we had 35 and 28 manuscript submissions related to artificial intelligence,
respectively. If our editorial board matches the manuscripts well, then we would
expect the first column’s frequencies to be in roughly equal proportions
to the last three columns. To a first approximation we find this to be true,
although there are some possible exceptions. Taking an arbitrary criterion of
the ratio of submissions to editorial board members exceeding 4 and a frequency
of more than 8, the following keywords are underrepresented by the editorial
board: biology, neuroscience, cognitive development, intelligent agents, motor
control, perception, and computational neuroscience. Two of these terms, neuroscience
and cognitive development, appeared on our list of underrepresented areas in
2002 and 2003 as well. At the very least, when the next editorial board is gathered
for 2006, particular attention should be paid to these areas.
A perennial concern for the journal and conference is that cognitive science
may be too dominated by psychologists. This is a concern not because of anything
intrinsically wrong with psychology, but because many people read the journal
to discover articles written for and by practitioners in a wide range of fields.
This worry is not alleviated by an inspection of the table. Of the main fields
that comprise cognitive science, psychology is by far the most often listed
by authors: psychology (listed as a keyword by 52% of submissions), linguistics
(15%), artificial intelligence (13%), neuroscience (10%), philosophy (8%), and
computer science (7%) [the full set of field percentages sum to more than 100%
because we do not restrict authors to designating only a single field]. I raised
the same concern in last year’s editor’s report, and received a
mixed response. Not all readers felt that the preponderance of psychology submissions
was a negative occurrence or that proactive measures should be taken to more
forcibly balance the submissions. I would be curious about other readers’
opinion, which I will pass along to my successor.
Fields Board
2001 2002
2003 2004
Anthropology
5
1 2 6
3
Artificial Intelligence 14 21
25 35 28
Biology
0 1
3 7
8
Computer Science 8
12
18 28 14
Education 9
10 8
13 7
Linguistics
12 13 23
37 32
Neuroscience 5
6 20
24 21
Philosophy 8
8 13 25
17
Psychology 31
69 113 127 109
Topics:
Analogy 10
3 14
9 5
Animal cognition 1
3 3
5 7
Attention 3
5 9
10
11
Artificial Life 4
3
1 1 3
Case-based reasoning 7
4 1
0 0
Causal reasoning 13
6 8
11 9
Cognitive architecture 16 16
24 32 29
Cognitive development 4
16 18
29 20
Communication 6
5 10
10 9
Complex systems 5
5 7
11 0
Computer vision 3
3 7
2 2
Concepts 20
14
16 31
19
Consciousness 5
1
4 13
8
Creativity 4
3 5
5 4
Culture 3
1
4 11 4
Decision making 9
11 15
19 8
Discourse 3
3 11
15 9
Distributed cognition 6
4
13 10
8
Emotion 3
2 0
6 3
Epistemology 4
6
8 11 4
Evolutionary psychology 3 4
3 8
9
Expertise 3
2
5 6 5
Human-computer interaction 7 4
5 7
6
Human factors 3
1 2 4
4
Information 1
3 5
11 7
Instruction 6
7 4
5 3
Intelligent agents 2
8 4
8 11
Language acquisition 12
13 12
11 12
Language understanding 12 6
24 37
23
Learning 14
25 25 35 24
Machine learning 10
7 11
11 8
Memory 6
13
20 30
13
Motor control 2
4 2
4 9
Music 1
0 1
1 1
Pattern recognition 6
7 11
12 8
Perception 4
12 23
22 25
Philosophy of computation 3 1
0 5
1
Philosophy of mind 6
8 8
19 11
Philosophy of science 8
3 5
16
3
Pragmatics 2
2 7 6
3
Problem Solving 9
21
12 19
12
Reasoning 14
19
22 29
23
Representation 26
25
41 42
32
Semantics 9
6
17 24
16
Situated cognition 4
4 8 9
11
Skill acquisition/learning 4 11
10 10 7
Social cognition 3
4
6 15 10
Speech recognition 1
2 1
5 3
Syntax 4
6 7 12
5
Translation 0
0 0
1 0
Methods:
Animal experimentation 1
1 3 1
3
Clinical methods 1
0 2 2
1
Computational neuroscience 2
1 6 9
9
Computer simulation 17
26 46 24 28
Cross-cultural analysis 3 0
1 5
3
Cross-linguistic analysis 3 0
2 1
1
Developmental experimentation 8 7
9 11
4
Electroencephalography (EEG) 2 0
2 0
4
Ethnography 1
0 0 2
2
Human experimentation 24
28 48
65 52
Knowledge representation 12
16 28 33
25
Logic 2
3 6
12
8
Mathematical modeling 8
13 23 22
10
Neural Networks 12
14 29
23 24
Neuro-imaging
2 2
2 3 3
Robotics 3
2 1
4 3
Single-cell recording 0
1 0
1
1
Statistics 8
7 5
13 7
Symbolic modeling 11
10
11 13 15
Another way of capturing the some of the thematic trends in Cognitive Science
is by a conceptual map. The concept map below reflects the ISI-assigned keywords
for all articles published in Cognitive Science from January 2001 to August
2004. The size of a node reflects the number of times it was given as a keyword
by an article. The thickness of a link shows the frequency with which two keywords
were paired together in articles. Only keywords listed by more than one article
appear in the map. This map was created by Weimao Ke and Katy Börner at
Indiana University, and I thank them for letting me reproduce it here. Among
other trends, the map shows the large role played by psychology and computational
models in the journal, and also the strong link between the two methods.
2E) Quantitative Measures of Importance
Quantitative estimates of a journal’s importance are notoriously controversial.
At the very least, they are important because people think they are important.
The most common reported measures of a journal’s importance are ISI’s
(http://www.isinet.com/isi/) Impact Factor and Cited Half-life. The two measures
complement each other because the former measures the short-term impact of a
journal’s articles while the latter measures long-term impact.
More specifically, the impact factor for a given year is calculated by dividing
the number of citations in the given year to articles published in the two previous
years by the total number of articles published in the two previous years. For
example, Cognitive Science’s 2003 impact factor is obtained by dividing
the number of citations to Cognitive Science articles appearing in 2001 and
2002 by the total number of articles appearing in 2001 and 2002.
Across all sciences, impact factor is assumed to be the most important factor
in assessing a journal. However, in my opinion, this should only be the case
for journals in fields that are so fast-moving that articles more than two years
old are only marginally relevant to current research. It is important not to
judge the field of cognitive science by criteria created for biomedical sciences
and molecular biology. After all, one of the core fields comprising cognitive
science is philosophy, where even 2300-year-old writings by Plato are considered
pertinent to contemporary theorizing.
Cognitive Science’s recent impact factors are as follows:
1997: 1.53
1998: 1.96
1999: 2.27
2000: 1.63
2001: 2.44
2002: 1.52
2003: 2.67
In last year’s report, I reported that the low 2002 impact factor was
due to a one-time change from our journal being published 4 times to 6 times
per year. Despite my distrust of the impact factor measure, I was relieved that
the one-time dip was indeed one-time. Our 2003 impact factor is the highest
that I have record for, and places us as #10 out of 67 journals in our ISI-designated
field of Experimental Psychology. Yes, I realize that this designation hardly
does justice to the broad range of topics covered by Cognitive Science, but
ISI insists on placing each journal into only a single category. Somebody should
inform ISI that science is bipartite graph, not a hierarchy. Sanctioning the
kind of cross-categorizations that ISI forbids, if we were in ISI’s category
“Artificial intelligence,” our impact factor ranking would be #11
out of 77 journals, #1 out of 92 journals listed under “Education and
Educational Research,” #2 out of 53 “Anthropology” journals,
and #6 out of 46 “Multidisciplinary Sciences.”
One of the obvious limitations of the impact factor measure is that citations
to articles that were published prior to 2001 have absolutely no influence on
a journal’s 2003 impact factor. Given this obvious blindspot of the impact
factor, it is useful to consider cited half-life, defined as the number of publication
years from the current year which account for 50% of current citations received.
For example, in 2003, 39% of the citations to Cognitive Science articles were
to articles appearing between 1994 and 2003. The remaining citations were to
Cognitive Science articles published at least 10 years prior to 2001. ISI has
a single category “>10” that is used for all journals with a
cited half-life of 10 years or more. With this long preamble out of the way,
here are the 2003 impact factors for Cognitive Science and some journals that
I selected as being within Cognitive Science’s peer group in a variety
of disciplines covered by ISI.
Journal
Impact Factor Cited Half-life
Adaptive Behavior
0.14 7.0
Artificial Intelligence 2.45
9.6
Brain and Language 1.32
8.8
Cognition 4.30
9.3
Cognitive Psychology 3.68
>10
Cognitive Science 2.67
>10
J. Artif. Intell. Res. 1.62
6.4
J. Cognitive Neuroscience 5.01
5.1
JEP:LMC 2.52
9.1
J. of Math. Psych. 0.74
>10
J. of Learning Science 1.60
7.4
Language 0.79
>10
Learning and Instruction 1.30 6.5
Machine Learning 3.05
7.4
Memory & Cognition 1.41
10.0
Minds & Machines 0.34
6.6
Neural Computation 2.75
6.4
Psychological Review 8.36
>10
In terms of these and other measures of influence, Cognitive Science looks very
good indeed. The downloads of articles from Elsevier’s on-line ScienceDirect
site were 5000, 12000, 20000, and 55588 in 2000, 2001, 2002, and 2003, and 19456
in the first four months of 2004, although this may speak more to the exponential
growth in electronic downloads of scholarly content than it does Cognitive Science’s
popularity per se. Still, in the first four months of 2004, the average number
of downloads per on-line article were an impressive 50. If you are curious,
the most downloaded Cognitive Science articles from 2003 were:
H. Cruse: “The evolution of cognition – a hypothesis”
R. Shiffrin: “Modeling memory and perception”
P. Verschure and P. Althaus: “A real-world rational agent: Unifying old
and new AI”
W. Schneider and J. Chein: “Controlled and automatic processing: Behavior,
theory, and biological mechanisms”
J. Waskan: “Intrinsic cognitive models”
S. Ainsworth and A. Loizou: “The effects of self-explaining when learning
with text and diagrams”
A. Norenzayan, E. Smith, B. Jun Kim, and R. Nisbett: “Cultural preferences
for formal versus intuitive reasoning”
S. Dasgupta: “Multidisciplinary creativity: The case of H. A. Simon”
3) Appreciation
In wrapping up this annual summary, I would like to personally thank our associate
editors, the board of reviewers, and our ever-growing assembly of ad-hoc reviewers.
The dedication, wisdom, and thoughtfulness of editors and reviewers drive the
journal, and is the basis for its continued vitality. I thank the authors for
considering Cognitive Science as an outlet for exciting developments, for their
receptivity to suggestions, for the patience that we have had to occasionally
tax, and for assuring that Cognitive Science will continue to stay on the forefront
of innovations in the interdisciplinary study of minds and other intelligent
systems. In 2004, the journal also benefited greatly from the professionalism,
expertise, and proactive attitude of Elsevier’s crew, including Fiona
Barron, Diana Jones, Niamh O’Hea, Elizabeth Purcell, and Mary Sheedy.
In my dealings with LEA, I have been greatly impressed with their crew’s
can-do spirit and creative problem solving, which includes Steven Cestaro, Jim
Conroy, Lawrence Erlbaum, Rick Maffei, Joseph Petrowski, Steve Rutberg, and
Nancy Seitz. James Greeno (the ex-editor of Cognitive Science), Bill Bechtel
(the past chair of the publications committee of the Cognitive Science Society),
Douglas Medin (the chair of new editor search committee, and hence my immanent
liberator), Art Markman (the ex-executive officer for the society and continued
spiritual guide), and Tom Ward (the current executive officer for the society,
whom I bothered no fewer than 6 times with queries bearing on this annual report
alone) have been very generous with their time, good ideas, know-how, and enthusiasm.
Finally, I am indebted to Caroline Verdier, the managing editor for the journal,
who has done a wonderful job of seeing to the daily care and feeding of the
journal, as well as being mindful of plans for its long-term development.
Best wishes for a Happy and Thoughtful 2005,
Rob Goldstone, Executive Editor of Cognitive Science